WWW.ABSTRACT.XLIBX.INFO
FREE ELECTRONIC LIBRARY - Abstract, dissertation, book
 
<< HOME
CONTACTS



Pages:   || 2 |

«Dual-mechanism morphology Harald Clahsen June 2004 Keywords: morphological processing, regular and irregular inflection, the past-tense debate, ...»

-- [ Page 1 ] --

In: K. Brown (ed.), (2006), Encyclopedia of language and linguistics. Elsevier:

Oxford, Vol. 4, pp.1-5.

Dual-mechanism morphology

Harald Clahsen

June 2004

Keywords: morphological processing, regular and irregular inflection, the past-tense

debate, storage versus computation, psycholinguistics, words and rules,

morphological decomposition, child morphology

Harald Clahsen

Department of Linguistics

University of Essex

Colchester, C04 3SQ, UK Tel: +44/1206/87-2228 Fax: +44/1206/87-2198 email: harald@essex.ac.uk Abstract Dual-mechanism morphology refers to a family of psycholinguistic models which hold that morphologically complex word forms can be processed both associatively, i.e. through stored full-form representations and by rules that decompose or parse inflected or derived word forms into morphological constituents. We present a brief overview of some relevant experimental results on English and other languages and of different variants of dual-mechanism models that are currently discussed in the literature. Finally, we briefly outline how dual-mechanism morphology can be implemented into morphological theory.

Our understanding of how an adult native speaker/hearer processes inflected word forms in real time has increased considerably over the last decade. Experimental studies using a range of different psycholinguistic methods and techniques, e.g.

lexical decision, priming, event-related potentials (ERPs), have led to a number of consistent and replicable results, e.g. frequency effects for inflected word forms in lexical decision tasks, priming effects for inflected word forms in different kinds of priming experiments, and left-anterior negativities for incorrectly inflected word forms in ERP violation studies; see Clahsen (1999) and Pinker (1999), for review.

The theoretical interpretation of these and other results on morphological processing in adult native speakers is controversial; basically, three different approaches can be distinguished. On one side are different kinds of associative single-mechanism models claiming that all inflected words are stored and processed within a single associative system using distributed representations (see e.g. Bybee 1995, Sereno & Jongman 1997, among others). The morphological structure of an inflected word is not explicitly represented in these models; instead, these models implement networks that represent the mapping relationship between different word forms through associatively linked orthographic, phonological and semantic codes. A radical alternative to these models are rule-based single-mechanism accounts in which all inflected words are formed by morphophonological rules, and memorization of inflected words is avoided as much as possible. Halle & Mohanan (1985) and related work in Generative Phonology in which minor rules are proposed, for example 'Lowering Ablaut' deriving the past-tense form sang from the stem sing, are representative of this approach; see Yang (2000) for a recent treatment of the English past tense in this framework. The third theoretical viewpoint is represented by a family of dual-mechanism models which hold that morphologically complex word forms can be processed both associatively, i.e. through stored full-form representations and by rules that decompose or parse inflected word forms into morphological constituents (Chialant & Caramazza 1995, Schreuder & Baayen 1995, Clahsen 1999, Pinker 1999, among others). Here, we will provide a brief overview of different variants of dual-mechanism models that are currently discussed in the literature.

There is a class of so-called dual-route models that is specifically concerned with the kinds of mental representation readers and listeners consult in recognizing morphologically complex words. As is common in word-recognition research, modality-specific access representations are assumed to mediate the mapping from the orthographic or spoken input onto lexical representations in the central lexicon. Dualroute models posit that morphologically complex words can be recognized either via prelexical morphological parsing which decomposes the orthographic or spoken input into its morphological components or via a direct access route which is based on stored full-form representations for morphologically complex words. Different proposals have been made as to how these two access routes interact. For example, according to Caramazza and collaborators, full-form based lexical access is preferred for all known words, and the parsing route is only employed for novel or extremely rare words that are morphologically regular (Chialant & Caramazza 1995). By contrast, according to the morphological race model proposed by Baayen, Schreuder and collaborators both the parsing route and the direct route operate in parallel. Which of these two routes turns out to be more efficient is claimed to depend on a number of factors, including the frequency of an inflected word and its constituents, the lexical neighborhood, the phonological and semantic transparency (vs. opaqueness) of a morphologically complex word, and the degree of homonymity of an affix (see e.g.

Baayen et al. 2003, Bertram et al. 2000). Much of the empirical evidence in the context of dual-route models comes from just one experimental task, namely so-called lexical decision, in which subjects have to discriminate between existing words (that have been encountered before) and nonce words (that have never been encountered before), e.g. houses vs. nouses. Lexical decision is a rather odd task, and it is hard to tell what response times to this task actually mean; see Balota (1994) and Deutsch & Müller (1999) for discussion. It seems clear, however, that due to the task demands, lexical decision encourages subjects to strongly rely on memory, in order to determine whether or not they have encountered a stimulus word before. Thus, it could be that results from lexical decision have lead us to overestimate the role of full-form access (as opposed to morphological parsing) in recognizing inflected and derived words.





Another variant of dual-mechanism morphology is the words-and-rules model of Pinker and collaborators (see e.g. Pinker 1999, Pinker & Ullman 2002). The basic claim of the words-and-rules account is that two complementary systems coexist in how our mind/brain processes and learns language: a combinatorial (rule-based) system that generalizes over symbolic categories such as N(oun), V(erb), etc. and treats all members of a given category equally, and an associative system that extracts probabilistic contingencies, e.g. frequency distributions, similarity clusters, etc. from the input data. These two systems can be implemented as neural circuits or subroutines which do not just underlie linguistic generalization, but are claimed to be used in other domains of cognition as well, for learning, and comparing in general.

The key property of the combinatorial system is that it suppresses differences between individual examples and treats all members of a group or class equally. By contrast, the associative system generalizes on the basis of resemblance to stored examples.

Extending Pinker’s model, Ullman (2001) has argued that the distinction between words and rules can be linked to two different brain memory systems, according to which the associative system (‘words’) depends upon declarative memory and is rooted in temporal lobe structures of the brain, whereas the combinatorial system (‘rules’) is rooted in frontal brain structures. However, the evidence currently available for such a strong localist interpretation of the words-and-rules distinction has not yet convinced everybody.

Much of the empirical research on the words-and-rules model comes from the study of a rather simple inflectional system, the English past tense. Pinker argues that the two different systems of his model directly correspond to the linguistic distinction between regular and irregular past-tense formation. Regular -ed inflection is predictable in form, readily applies to novel items, and invokes a combinatory rule (Add -ed) that may operate on the outputs of other morphological processes (derivation, compounding). Irregular past-tense inflection (e.g. sing-sang) on the other hand, is based on stored forms that cannot be perfectly predicted by the form of the stem or root, and only tentatively extends to new forms. There is indeed a considerable body of psycholinguistic evidence for the distinctions posited by the words-and-rules model from studies of the English past tense, investigating child language acquisition (Marcus et al. 1992), adult language processing (Alegre & Gordon 1999), brain-imaging and event-related potentials (Jaeger et al. 1996, Münte et al. 1999), and language disorders (Marslen-Wilson & Tyler 1997, Clahsen et al.

2004).

However, despite all the evidence that has been accumulated on the English past tense, there are many reasons to extend research on dual-mechanism morphology to a wider range of languages. Note, for example, that the English past tense has only one productive form (-ed) and that regularity is confounded with both the presence of an overt affix and with type frequency. Regular verbs in English are much more frequent (= 95%) than irregular ones (= 5%), see Marcus et al. (1995), and regular past tense forms contain a segmentable affix, whereas irregular forms typically do not have affixes. These two confounding features leave room for several alternative interpretations of the same set of facts. Potential differences, for example, between forms such as walk-ed and came in acquisition and processing could be effects of frequency differences and/or effects of the presence or absence of an overt affix, rather than the result of different mental representations.

To address these concerns, psycholinguistic studies have examined a range of typologically different languages from the perspective of dual-mechanism morphology (e.g. German: Clahsen (1999); Hebrew: Berent et al. (1999); Italian: Say & Clahsen (2002); Spanish: Clahsen et al. (2002a), Rodriguez-Fornells et al. (2002);

Catalan: Rodriguez-Fornells et al. (2001)) focusing on the question of whether and how the fundamental distinction between lexical storage and combinatory rules extends to linguistic systems other than the English past tense (see Pinker 1999, chapter 8 for review). The results of these studies revealed contrasts that were similar to those obtained for regular and irregular inflection in English despite the fact that the vocabulary distribution, the surface morphology, and other properties of the language were different from those of English past-tense formation. For example, psycholinguistic examination of the German noun plural system revealed that even though the -s plural is the least frequent plural allomorph in German, it exhibits the same experimental effects as the regular past-tense -ed in English (Marcus et al.

1995). The same is true for past participle formation in German despite the fact that (unlike English) German has two segmentable participle endings, the regular -t and the irregular -n, and that the vocabulary distribution for regulars and irregulars is different from that of the English past tense (Clahsen 1999). Priming experiments revealed regular/irregular differences for German participles despite the fact that the phonological and orthographical distance between the participle forms and their corresponding stem forms tested was identical for both regular and irregular verbs (Sonnenstuhl et al. 1999). For example, an irregular participle such as geschlafen ‘slept’ is as similar to its base form (schlaf) as a regular participle (e.g. geöffnet ‘opened’) is to its base stem (öffn-), and despite these formal similarities the regular form produced full priming, and the irregular one did not, just like in corresponding experiments on the English past tense (Stanners et al. 1979). These results do not only show that the words-and-rules model extends to morphological systems other than the English past tense, but also that alternative non-morphological factors (e.g. the frequency distribution and the surface properties of inflected word forms) that may hold for the English past tense cannot explain the observed contrasts between lexically-based (‘frozen’) and rule-based (‘built’) inflected word forms.

There are different ways of how dual-mechanism morphology can be implemented into an explicit linguistic theory. One direct implementation is Wunderlich and colleagues’ morpheme-based model (Wunderlich 1996) according to which irregularly inflected forms are lexically represented, as subnodes of structured lexical entries, while regular inflection involves combinatorial rules that concatenate an affix with a lexical entry. An alternative implementation within realization-based models of morphology reconstructs this contrast in terms of the distinction between rules that contain variables and those that have a constant output, see e.g. Blevins (2004).

Irregular inflection, e.g. the form is of the verb to be, is encoded as a realization rule

–  –  –

combinatorial (regular) inflection, e.g. the regular 3sg present tense -s in English, is based on a realization rule ([V, 3sg, pres, ind], X+s) that adds the exponent ‘s’ to the base form represented by a variable (‘X’) yielding inflected word forms for any verb that is not blocked by a more specific rule. Thus, although dual-mechanism morphology can be construed in terms of an opposition between rules and entries, the basic distinction between built and stored elements can also be expressed in alternative all-rules models of morphology.



Pages:   || 2 |


Similar works:

«A Békés Megyei Múzeumok Közleményei б—1980 Ökológiai és faunisztikai összefoglalás a szarvasi arboretum madárvilágáról 1956—1965 VERTSE ALBERT A szarvasi arborétumban 1956-tól 1965-ig minden évben eltöltöttem egy—két hetet madártelepítési munkálatokkal. Ez idő alatt végzett faunisztikai és ökológiai meg­ figyeléseimet foglalom össze az alábbiakban. Elöljáróként megemlítem, hogy a magyarországi parkokról jóformán nem jelent meg eddig madártani...»

«Kasem Schariatmadari 1 Kasem Schariatmadari Großajatollah Mohammad Kasem Schariatmadari auch Kazem Shari'atmadari(* 1905 in Tabriz; † 1986), persisch ‫مظاک‌دمحم‏‬ ‫یرادمتعیرش‬‎), war vor der islamischen Revolution der einflussreichste iranische Großajatollah. Schariatmaderi vertrat die Meinung, dass Geistliche keine politischen Ämter übernehmen und sich auch nicht unmittelbar politisch betätigen sollten. Er vertrat damit die Linie, die heute von Ali al...»

«Diplomarbeit Titel der Diplomarbeit Untersuchung zur Bedeutung der elterlichen Mentalisierungsfähigkeit und der Bindungssicherheit des Kindes für die Entwicklung der Theory of Mind und der Reflexiven Kompetenz von Kindern im Vorschulalter. Verfasserin Lisa Müller Angestrebter akademischer Grad Magistra der Naturwissenschaften (Mag. rer. nat.) Wien, am 06.03.2013 Studienkennzahl lt. Studienblatt: A298 Studienrichtung lt. Studienblatt: Psychologie Betreuerin: ao. Univ.-Prof. Dr. Mag. Ulrike...»

«HUNG. KÖZL. 17. ÉVF. 3-4. (64-65.) SZ. 281-96. L. NOVI SAD ÚJVIDÉK 1985. JÚNIUS-SZEPTEMBER ETO: 392.51 CONFERENCE PAPER VŐFÉLYEK ÉS VŐFÉLYVERSEK SZEREPE A TOPOLYAI ÉS KÖRNYÉKI LAKODALMI SZOKÁSOKBAN Borús Rózsa Nyugalmazott tisztviselő, Topolya Közlésre elfogadva: 1985. december 10. A Topolyai vőfélykönyv rendelkezésemre álló anyagát 1975-1977 között gyűjtöttem össze. Meglepődve tapasztaltam, mennyire népszerűek e rigmusok, milyen sok a változat, különösen...»

«A Z APENTA KESERŰVÍZ KUTATÁSTÖRTÉNETE DOBOS I R M A A keserűvíztelepek kialakulása A z Ő r m e z e i v ö l g y b e n a kutak és a telepek létesítését minden bizonnyal Saxlehner András 1 863-ban az ö r s ö d i területen feltárt és palackozott Hunyadi János keserűvíz korán fellendült forgalma segítette elő. A két terület közel azonos földtani felépítése m é g jobban a l á t á m a s z ­ totta az e r e d m é n y e s vízfeltárást és annak nyomán a remélt...»

«Studies in Humanities and Social Sciences, Volume 11, Indian Institute of Advanced Study, Shimla, RITUAL, MASKS, AND SACRIFICE Subhash Kak 1 INTRODUCTION Ritual is intimately connected with the mask, either in the wearing that hides the true face, or in the adoption of a public face. The mask makes the disengagement from ordinary time and the connection to the ancient and repetitive, which is the heart of ritual, psychologically acceptable. Together, ritual and mask facilitate the apprehension...»

«SÁNDORFI JÓZSEF (17 6 7 — 1 8 2 4) BATÁRI GYULA A magyar orvostörténet sok olyan kiváló orvos nevét jegyezte fel, aki a hivatásán kívül még más területen is jelentős tevékenységet fejtett k i, de kevés orvosról tudunk, aki annyi mindennel foglalkozott volna eredményesen, mint Sándorfi József. Orvostudomány, költészet, táblabíráskodás, írói tevékenység, műfordítás, nyelvművelés, zene, szerkesztői munka, tudomány-népszerűsítés, könyvgyűjtés és...»

«Diplomarbeit Titel der Diplomarbeit Power and Trust in Organizations: Testing a mediation model to analyze their effects on Organizational Citizenship Behavior and Deviant Workplace Behavior Verfasser Gil Multhaupt Angestrebter akademischer Grad Magister der Naturwissenschaften (Mag. rer. nat.) Köln, 2012 Studienkennzahl: 298 Studienrichtung: Psychologie Betreuer: Univ.-Prof. Dr. Erich Kirchler Danksagung Ich möchte mich an dieser Stelle ganz besonders bei meiner Familie bedanken, die mich...»

«DIPLOMARBEIT Titel der Diplomarbeit „Lernendenautonomie und ihre Grenzen in DaF-/DaZ-Kursen mit Erwachsenen“ Verfasserin Manuela Wascher angestrebter akademischer Grad Magistra der Philosophie (Mag. phil.) Wien, 2008 Studienkennzahl lt. A 333 344 Studienblatt: Studienrichtung lt. Deutsche Philologie Studienblatt: Betreuer: Univ.-Prof. Dr. Klaus-Börge Boeckmann Inhaltsverzeichnis 1 Einleitung 1 2 Konzeptualisierungen von „Lernendenautonomie“ 4 2.1 Selbstgesteuertes Lernen ohne...»

«Földrajzi Értesítő 2002. LI. évf. 1-2. füzet, pp. 167-183. Logisztikai központok elhelyezkedése és működési jellemzői a magyar közlekedési hálózatban SCHUBERT GÉZA1 Zusammenfassung Die Lage und die Funktionsweise von Logistikzentren im ungarischen Verkehrsnetz Angesichts des tagtäglich anwachsenden Güterverkehrs auf den Straßen rückt eine Lösung dieses Problems auch in Ungarn immer mehr ins Rampenlicht. Aufgrund des zentralistisch ausgerichteten sternförmigen...»

«001_0002.QXD_001_0002.QXD 19.03.12 16:27 Seite 1 ASSOCIATION INTERNATIONALE POUR LA PROTECTION DE LA PROPRIETE INTELLECTUELLE INTERNATIONAL ASSOCIATION FOR THE PROTECTION OF INTELLECTUAL PROPERTY INTERNATIONALE VEREINIGUNG FÜR DEN SCHUTZ DES GEISTIGEN EIGENTUMS YEARBOOK 2003 / I MEETING OF THE EXECUTIVE COMMITTEE L U C E R N E 2 0 0 3 ( O C T O B E R 2 5 – 2 8, 2 0 03 ) Q 17 3 : ISSUES OF CO-EXISTENCE OF TRADEMARKS AND DOMAIN NAMES: PUBLIC VERSUS PRIVATE INTERNATIONAL REGISTRATION SYSTEMS Q...»

«COMMITMENT TO THE EAST AFRICAN COMMUNITY CUSTOMS UNION PROTOCOL, 2004-2009 BY PAUL WAMBI BAGABO A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE UNIVERSITY OF BIRMINGHAM FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY INTERNATIONAL DEVELOPMENT DEPARTMENT SCHOOL OF GOVERNMENT AND SOCIETY UNIVERSITY OF BIRMINGHAM JUNE 2012 University of Birmingham Research Archive e-theses repository This unpublished thesis/dissertation is copyright of the author and/or third parties. The intellectual property rights of the author or third...»





 
<<  HOME   |    CONTACTS
2016 www.abstract.xlibx.info - Free e-library - Abstract, dissertation, book

Materials of this site are available for review, all rights belong to their respective owners.
If you do not agree with the fact that your material is placed on this site, please, email us, we will within 1-2 business days delete him.